What was zimbardos trying to prove
An article ran in the Times Magazine in April of In various ways, these accounts reiterated the claim that relatively small changes in circumstances could turn the best and brightest into monsters or depersonalized serfs. He served as a consultant on the new film, which follows his original study in detail, relying on direct transcripts from the experimental recordings and taking few dramatic liberties. In many ways, the film is critical of the study: Crudup plays Zimbardo as an overzealous researcher overstepping his bounds, trying to create a very specific outcome among the students he observes.
The filmmakers even underscore the flimsiness of the experimental design, inserting characters who point out that Zimbardo is not a disinterested observer. Based on my experience, and what I saw and what I felt, I think that was a real stretch.
If the Stanford Prison Experiment had simulated a less brutal environment, would the prisoners and guards have acted differently? Their guards also had uniforms, and were given latitude to dole out rewards and punishments; their prisoners were placed in three-person cells that followed the layout of the Stanford County Jail almost exactly. The main difference was that, in this prison, the preset expectations were gone.
The BBC Prison Study, as it came to be called, differed from the Stanford experiment in a few other ways, including prisoner dress; for a while, moreover, the prisoners were told that they could become guards through good behavior, although, on the third day, that offer was revoked, and the roles were made permanent.
The prisoners, on the other hand, developed a collective identity. In a change from the Stanford study, the psychologists asked each participant to complete a daily survey that measured the degree to which he felt solidarity with his group; it showed that, as the guards grew further apart, the prisoners were growing closer together. On the fourth day, three cellmates decided to test their luck.
At lunchtime, one threw his plate down and demanded better food, another asked to smoke, and the third asked for medical attention for a blister on his foot. It also provides evidence that the conclusions of the experiment were predetermined. Many of the classic show-stopping experiments in psychology have lately turned out to be wrong, fraudulent, or outdated.
The Zimbardo prison experiment is not the only classic study that has been recently scrutinized, reevaluated, or outright exposed as a fraud. Perry has also revealed inconsistencies in another major early work in psychology: the Milgram electroshock test, in which participants were told by an authority figure to deliver seemingly lethal doses of electricity to an unseen hapless soul. Her investigations show some evidence of researchers going off the study script and possibly coercing participants to deliver the desired results.
Other studies have been reevaluated for more honest, methodological snafus. I can list so many more textbook psychology findings that have either not replicated, or are currently in the midst of a serious reevaluation.
Alas, the past few years have brought about a reckoning for these ideas and social psychology as a whole. Many psychological theories have been debunked or diminished in rigorous replication attempts. Psychologists are now realizing it's more likely that false positives will make it through to publication than inconclusive results. For instance, it used to be commonplace for scientists to publish experiments that sampled about 50 undergraduate students. Today, scientists realize this is a recipe for false positives , and strive for sample sizes in the hundreds and ideally from a more representative subject pool.
Nevertheless, in so many of these cases, scientists have moved on and corrected errors, and are still doing well-intentioned work to understand the heart of humanity.
In some cases, time has shown that flawed original experiments offer worthwhile reexamination. The original Milgram experiment was flawed. But at least its study design — which brings in participants to administer shocks not actually carried out to punish others for failing at a memory test — is basically repeatable today with some ethical tweaks. Replication attempts at the Stanford prison study, on the other hand, have been a mess. In science, too often, the first demonstration of an idea becomes the lasting one — in both pop culture and academia.
Science is a frustrating, iterative process. When we communicate it, we need to get beyond the idea that a single, stunning study ought to last the test of time. Scientists know this as well, but their institutions have often discouraged them from replicating old work, instead of the pursuit of new and exciting, attention-grabbing studies. Within a very short time both guards and prisoners were settling into their new roles, with the guards adopting theirs quickly and easily. Within hours of beginning the experiment some guards began to harass prisoners.
At A. The counts served as a way to familiarizing the prisoners with their numbers. More importantly, they provided a regular occasion for the guards to exercise control over the prisoners. The prisoners soon adopted prisoner-like behavior too. They talked about prison issues a great deal of the time. Some even began siding with the guards against prisoners who did not obey the rules. The prisoners were taunted with insults and petty orders, they were given pointless and boring tasks to accomplish, and they were generally dehumanized.
Push-ups were a common form of physical punishment imposed by the guards. One of the guards stepped on the prisoners' backs while they did push-ups, or made other prisoners sit on the backs of fellow prisoners doing their push-ups.
Because the first day passed without incident, the guards were surprised and totally unprepared for the rebellion which broke out on the morning of the second day. During the second day of the experiment, the prisoners removed their stocking caps, ripped off their numbers, and barricaded themselves inside the cells by putting their beds against the door. The guards called in reinforcements. The three guards who were waiting on stand-by duty came in and the night shift guards voluntarily remained on duty.
The guards retaliated by using a fire extinguisher which shot a stream of skin-chilling carbon dioxide, and they forced the prisoners away from the doors. Next, the guards broke into each cell, stripped the prisoners naked and took the beds out. The ringleaders of the prisoner rebellion were placed into solitary confinement.
After this, the guards generally began to harass and intimidate the prisoners. One of the three cells was designated as a "privilege cell. The guards gave them back their uniforms and beds and allowed them to wash their hair and brush their teeth.
Privileged prisoners also got to eat special food in the presence of the other prisoners who had temporarily lost the privilege of eating. The effect was to break the solidarity among prisoners. Over the next few days, the relationships between the guards and the prisoners changed, with a change in one leading to a change in the other.
Remember that the guards were firmly in control and the prisoners were totally dependent on them. As the prisoners became more dependent, the guards became more derisive towards them. They held the prisoners in contempt and let the prisoners know it.
As the prisoners became more submissive, the guards became more aggressive and assertive. They demanded ever greater obedience from the prisoners. The prisoners were dependent on the guards for everything so tried to find ways to please the guards, such as telling tales on fellow prisoners.
Less than 36 hours into the experiment, Prisoner began suffering from acute emotional disturbance, disorganized thinking, uncontrollable crying, and rage. You can't quit. The next day, the guards held a visiting hour for parents and friends. They were worried that when the parents saw the state of the jail, they might insist on taking their sons home.
Guards washed the prisoners, had them clean and polish their cells, fed them a big dinner and played music on the intercom. After the visit, rumor spread of a mass escape plan. Afraid that they would lose the prisoners, the guards and experimenters tried to enlist the help and facilities of the Palo Alto police department. Take our 5-minute anger test to find out if you're angry! Windows to the Soul What can a person's eyes tell you about what they are thinking?
Are You Stressed? Measure your stress levels with this 5-minute stress test. Memory Like A Goldfish? Take Psychologist World's 5-minute memory test to measure your memory.
Slave To Your Role? To what extent are people controlled by their roles in society? Are You Fixated? Discover your Freudian personality type with our Fixation Test. Interpret Your Dreams Learn to interpret the hidden meanings behind the themes of your dreams and nightmares. How to Read Body Language Learn to read and understand body signals and improve your own body language.
How to Beat Stress and Succeed in Exams If you're one of the many people who gets stressed out when it comes to taking exams then we have a few tips for you that will help you to overcome this and really concentrating on achieving good grades. How Theodor Adorno's F-scale aimed to identify fascism and authoritarian Authoritarian Personality. How false memories are created and can affect our ability to recall events. What causes us to obey to authority figures such as police, teachers and A study reveals how the Big Five personality dimensions influence audiences' More on Influence.
Psychology approaches, theories and studies explained.
0コメント